المرجع الالكتروني للمعلوماتية
المرجع الألكتروني للمعلوماتية

English Language
عدد المواضيع في هذا القسم 6137 موضوعاً
Grammar
Linguistics
Reading Comprehension

Untitled Document
أبحث عن شيء أخر المرجع الالكتروني للمعلوماتية
القيمة الغذائية للثوم Garlic
2024-11-20
العيوب الفسيولوجية التي تصيب الثوم
2024-11-20
التربة المناسبة لزراعة الثوم
2024-11-20
البنجر (الشوندر) Garden Beet (من الزراعة الى الحصاد)
2024-11-20
الصحافة العسكرية ووظائفها
2024-11-19
الصحافة العسكرية
2024-11-19

الخصائص العامة لكوكب زحل
رسالته الذهبية في الطب
30-7-2016
القيام عند سماع اسمه الكريم(عجل الله فرجه)
3-08-2015
الولد والمجتمع / الرضا بما قسم الله تعالى
2024-07-10
سيرَة الأَنبياء وَالأَوصياء في طَلَب الرزق
23-12-2016
ما يخلفه الإنسان بعد موته
26-11-2014

Conditioned variation in written language  
  
769   11:17 صباحاً   date: 12-3-2022
Author : April Mc Mahon
Book or Source : An introduction of English phonology
Page and Part : 13-2


Read More
Date: 2024-05-01 634
Date: 2024-04-13 544
Date: 2024-06-18 429

Conditioned variation in written language

Since we are more used to thinking explicitly about written language than about our speech, one way of approaching this issue of abstraction is through our conscious knowledge of the rules of writing. When children learn to write, they have to master the conventions governing the use of capital and lower-case letters. Children often tend to learn to write their name before anything else, and this will have an initial capital; and children are also great generalizers, and indeed over-generalizers; for instance, first words often have a much wider range of meanings than their adult equivalents. Thus, for a one-year-old, cat may mean ‘any animal’ (whether real, toy, or picture), tractor ‘any vehicle’, and Daddy ‘any male adult’; these broad senses are later progressively narrowed down. It follows that children may at first try to write all words with initial capitals, until they are taught the accepted usage, which in modern English is for capitals to appear on proper names, I, and the first word in each sentence, and lower-case letters elsewhere, giving the prescribed patterns in (1).

Precisely how the capital and lower-case letters are written by an individual is not relevant, as long as they are recognizable and consistently distinct from other letters – an needs to be distinguished from on, and An from In, but it does not especially matter whether we find a or a for lower-case, and A  or A for capital; it all depends who we copy when we first learn, what our writing instruments and our grip on them are like, or typographically, which of the burgeoning range of fonts we fancy

Again, we seem readily able to perceive that all these subtly different variants can be grouped into classes. There is a set of lower-case and a set of capital letters, and the rules governing their distribution relate to those classes as units, regardless of the particular form produced on a certain occasion of writing. Moreover, the lower-case and capital sets together belong to a single, higher-order unit: they are all forms, or realizations of ‘the letter a’, an ideal and abstract unit to which we mentally compare and assign actual written forms. ‘The letter a’ never itself appears on paper, but it is conceptually real for us as users of the alphabet: this abstract unit is a grapheme, symbolized>a<  ; triangle brackets are conventionally used for spellings.

The choice of symbol is purely conventional: since it is a conceptual unit, and since we do not know what units look like in the brain, we might as well use an arbitrary sign likeor give it a nameis Annie Apple in the children’s Letterland series for beginning readers. However, it is convenient to use a form that looks like one of the actual realizations, as this will help us to match up the abstract grapheme with the actual graphs which manifest it in actual writing.

The rules governing the distribution of and other graphemes are not, however, absolute natural laws. Learning that proper names and sentences begin with capitals is appropriate for a child writing modern English, but not for a child learning German, who would need to learn instead that all nouns (not just Anna and Afrika but also Apfel ‘apple’) always begin with a capital letter, as well as all sentences. A similar strong tendency is observable in earlier stages of English too, and although literary style is not absolutely consistent in this respect, there are many more capitals in the work of a poet like John Milton, for instance, than in written English today; see (2).