المرجع الالكتروني للمعلوماتية
المرجع الألكتروني للمعلوماتية

English Language
عدد المواضيع في هذا القسم 6137 موضوعاً
Grammar
Linguistics
Reading Comprehension

Untitled Document
أبحث عن شيء أخر المرجع الالكتروني للمعلوماتية
القيمة الغذائية للثوم Garlic
2024-11-20
العيوب الفسيولوجية التي تصيب الثوم
2024-11-20
التربة المناسبة لزراعة الثوم
2024-11-20
البنجر (الشوندر) Garden Beet (من الزراعة الى الحصاد)
2024-11-20
الصحافة العسكرية ووظائفها
2024-11-19
الصحافة العسكرية
2024-11-19

William Trail
30-6-2016
أوروبا والفكر السياسي في خدمة الحكم المطلق.
2024-09-24
المضايق البحرية (مضيق جبل طارق)
13-4-2016
Sylvestre François Lacroix
9-7-2016
SULFUR
6-8-2017
اتفاقيات النقل الجوي الدولي
20/9/2022

More elaborate word forms: multiple affixation  
  
790   08:49 صباحاً   date: 2024-02-03
Author : Andrew Carstairs-McCarthy
Book or Source : An Introduction To English Morphology
Page and Part : 72-7


Read More
Date: 2024-02-05 967
Date: 2023-07-29 794
Date: 21-1-2022 1226

More elaborate word forms: multiple affixation

Many derived words contain more than one affix. Examples are unhelpfulness and helplessness. Imagine now that the structure of these words is entirely ‘flat’: that is, that they each consist of merely a string of affixes plus a root, no portions of the string being grouped together as a sub-string or smaller constituent within the word. An unfortunate consequence of that analysis is that it would complicate considerably what needs to be said about the behavior of the suffixes -ful and -less. These were straightforwardly treated as suffixes that attach to nouns to form adjectives. However, if the nouns unhelpfulness and helplessness are flat-structured, we must also allow -ful and -less to appear internally in a string that constitutes a noun – but not just anywhere in such a string, because (for example) the imaginary nouns *sadlessness and *meanlessingness, though they contain -less, are nevertheless not words, and (one feels) could never be words.

 

The flat-structure approach misses a crucial observation. Unhelpfulness contains the suffix -ful only by virtue of the fact that it contains (in some sense) the adjective helpful. Likewise, helplessness contains -less by virtue of the fact that it contains helpless. Once that is recognized, the apparent need to make special provision for -ful and -less when they appear inside complex words, rather than as their rightmost element, disappears. In fact, both these words can be seen as built up from the root help by successive processes of affixation (with N, V and A standing for noun, verb and adjective respectively):

(1) help N + -ful helpfulA

     un- + helpfulunhelpfulA

     unhelpful + -nessunhelpfulness N

(2) help N + -lesshelplessA

     helpless + -nesshelplessness N

 

Another way of representing this information is in terms of a branching tree diagram, as in (3) and (4), which also represent the fact that the noun help is formed by conversion from the verb:

(The term ‘tree diagram’ is odd, because the ‘branches’ point downwards, more like roots than branches! However, this topsy-turvy usage has become well established in linguistic discussions.) The points in a tree diagram from which branches sprout are called nodes. The nodes in (3) and (4) are all labelled, to indicate the wordclass of the string (that is, of the part of the whole word) that is dominated by the node in question. For example, the second-to-top node in (3) is labelled ‘A’ to indicate that the string unhelpful that it dominates is an adjective, while the topmost node is labelled ‘N’ because the whole word is a noun. The information about structure contained in tree diagrams such as (3) and (4) can also be conveyed in a labelled bracketing, where one pair of brackets corresponds to each node in the tree: [[un-[[helpV]N-ful]A]A-ness]N, [[[helpV]N -less]A-ness]N.

 

One thing stands out about all the nodes in (3) and (4): each has no more than two branches sprouting downwards from it. This reflects the fact that, in English, derivational processes operate by adding no more than one affix to a base – unlike languages where material may be added simultaneously at both ends, constituting what is sometimes called a circumfix. English possesses no uncontroversial examples of circumfixes, and branching within word-structure tree diagrams is never more than binary (i.e. with two branches). (The only plausible candidate for a circumfix in English is the en- …-en combination that forms enliven and embolden from live and bold; but en- and -en each appears on its own too, e.g. in enfeeble and redden, so an alternative analysis as a combination of a prefix and a suffix seems preferable.) The single branch connecting N to V above help in (3) and (4) reflects the fact that the noun help is derived from the verb help by conversion, with no affix.

 

At (5) and (6) are two more word tree diagrams, incorporating an adverbial (Adv) node and also illustrating both affixal and non-affixal heads, each italicized element being the head of the constituent dominated by the node immediately above it:

Some complex words contain elements about which one may reasonably argue whether they are complex or not. For example, the word reflection is clearly divisible into a base reflect and a suffix -ion; but does reflect itself consist of one morpheme or two? If we put it on one side, then any complex word form consisting of a free root and affixes turns out to be readily analyzable in the simple fashion illustrated here, with binary branching and with either the affix or the base as the head. (I say ‘free root’ rather than ‘root’ only because some bound roots are hard to assign to a wordclass: for example, matern- in maternal and maternity.)

 

Another salient point in all of (3)–(6) is that more than one node in a tree diagram may carry the same wordclass label (N, V, A). At first sight, this may not seem particularly remarkable. However, it has considerable implications for the size of the class of all possible words in English. Linguists are fond of pointing out that there is no such thing as the longest sentence of English (or of any language), because any candidate for longest-sentence status can be lengthened by embedding it in a context such as Sharon says that ___ . One cannot so easily demonstrate that there is no such thing as the longest word in English; but it is not necessary to do so in order to demonstrate the versatility and vigour of English word-formation processes. Given that we can find nouns inside nouns, verbs inside verbs, and so on, it is hardly surprising that the vocabulary of English, or of any individual speaker, is not a closed, finite list.