Grammar
Tenses
Present
Present Simple
Present Continuous
Present Perfect
Present Perfect Continuous
Past
Past Continuous
Past Perfect
Past Perfect Continuous
Past Simple
Future
Future Simple
Future Continuous
Future Perfect
Future Perfect Continuous
Passive and Active
Parts Of Speech
Nouns
Countable and uncountable nouns
Verbal nouns
Singular and Plural nouns
Proper nouns
Nouns gender
Nouns definition
Concrete nouns
Abstract nouns
Common nouns
Collective nouns
Definition Of Nouns
Verbs
Stative and dynamic verbs
Finite and nonfinite verbs
To be verbs
Transitive and intransitive verbs
Auxiliary verbs
Modal verbs
Regular and irregular verbs
Action verbs
Adverbs
Relative adverbs
Interrogative adverbs
Adverbs of time
Adverbs of place
Adverbs of reason
Adverbs of quantity
Adverbs of manner
Adverbs of frequency
Adverbs of affirmation
Adjectives
Quantitative adjective
Proper adjective
Possessive adjective
Numeral adjective
Interrogative adjective
Distributive adjective
Descriptive adjective
Demonstrative adjective
Pronouns
Subject pronoun
Relative pronoun
Reflexive pronoun
Reciprocal pronoun
Possessive pronoun
Personal pronoun
Interrogative pronoun
Indefinite pronoun
Emphatic pronoun
Distributive pronoun
Demonstrative pronoun
Pre Position
Preposition by function
Time preposition
Reason preposition
Possession preposition
Place preposition
Phrases preposition
Origin preposition
Measure preposition
Direction preposition
Contrast preposition
Agent preposition
Preposition by construction
Simple preposition
Phrase preposition
Double preposition
Compound preposition
Conjunctions
Subordinating conjunction
Correlative conjunction
Coordinating conjunction
Conjunctive adverbs
Interjections
Express calling interjection
Grammar Rules
Preference
Requests and offers
wishes
Be used to
Some and any
Could have done
Describing people
Giving advices
Possession
Comparative and superlative
Giving Reason
Making Suggestions
Apologizing
Forming questions
Since and for
Directions
Obligation
Adverbials
invitation
Articles
Imaginary condition
Zero conditional
First conditional
Second conditional
Third conditional
Reported speech
Linguistics
Phonetics
Phonology
Semantics
Pragmatics
Linguistics fields
Syntax
Morphology
Semantics
pragmatics
History
Writing
Grammar
Phonetics and Phonology
Reading Comprehension
Elementary
Intermediate
Advanced
Teaching Methods
Teaching Strategies
Double onsets
المؤلف:
Mehmet Yavas̡
المصدر:
Applied English Phonology
الجزء والصفحة:
P140-C6
2025-03-12
232
Double onsets
Table 1 shows the occurring double onsets of English. We can make the following observations. Affricates are the only class of consonants that do not appear in onset clusters. Besides this general statement, there are several other restrictions for two-member onset clusters:
• No voiced fricatives can serve as C2. Only /v/ can be a C1, and it can combine only with /j/ (e.g. view).
• No non-lateral approximant (/ɹ̣, w, j/) can serve as C1; the lateral can only precede /j/ (only for some speakers).
• No voiced stop can serve as C2.
• No fricative other than /f/ can serve as C2, and this can only be preceded by a /s/ in rarely found vocabulary (e.g. sphere).
• No stops or nasals are allowed as C2, except after /s/ (e.g. speak, small).
• /s/ and /ʃ/ are complementary: /s/ does not occur before /ɹ̣/, and /ʃ/ occurs only before /ɹ̣/ (e.g. shrimp).
• /h/ and /m/ can only occur before /j/ (e.g. huge, music).
• /θ/ can precede only /ɹ̣/ and /w/ (e.g. three, thwart).
• Labials (C1) do not cluster with a labial approximant.
• No geminates (i.e. doubled consonant sounds) are allowed.
• Alveolar stops (C1) do not cluster with /l/.
We can summarize the situation in the following manner. In general, English double onsets are either (a) /s/ +C (where C =any consonant that can assume the position of C2 except /ɹ̣/; /ʃ/ appears before /ɹ̣/), and (b) obstruent +approximant, with the limitations cited above.
While the pattern of C1 as an obstruent and C2 as a sonorant is very common, we do not have any double onset in which the reverse (C1 =sonorant and C2 =obstruent) is true. This pattern that we observe for English is also commonly found in many other languages, and can be accounted for by the principle known as ‘sonority sequencing’. We referred to sonority earlier for syllable peaks, and now we make reference to it for the sequencing of sounds with respect to a syllable peak by the Sonority Sequencing Principle (SSP). As given by Selkirk (1984: 116), SSP states that:
In any syllable, there is a segment constituting a sonority peak that is preceded and/or followed by a sequence of segments with progressively decreasing sonority values.
Thus, the expected pattern is that, going from C1 to C2, the sonority level will rise. Such is the case in the overwhelming majority of English double onsets (e.g. play[ple], cry[kɹ̣aɪ], quick[kwɪk]). The violations of this principle are /s/ +stop clusters (/sp, st, sk/), in which the sonority level drops, instead of rises, going from C1 to C2. As we will see with triple onsets as well as with double and triple codas, /s/ behaves exceptionally. To account for such cases, several scholars have suggested a special ‘adjunct’ status for /s/ clusters. We will not go into the details of such a proposal, but suffice it to state that this exceptional behavior of /s/ is also found in several other languages.