Grammar
Tenses
Present
Present Simple
Present Continuous
Present Perfect
Present Perfect Continuous
Past
Past Continuous
Past Perfect
Past Perfect Continuous
Past Simple
Future
Future Simple
Future Continuous
Future Perfect
Future Perfect Continuous
Passive and Active
Parts Of Speech
Nouns
Countable and uncountable nouns
Verbal nouns
Singular and Plural nouns
Proper nouns
Nouns gender
Nouns definition
Concrete nouns
Abstract nouns
Common nouns
Collective nouns
Definition Of Nouns
Verbs
Stative and dynamic verbs
Finite and nonfinite verbs
To be verbs
Transitive and intransitive verbs
Auxiliary verbs
Modal verbs
Regular and irregular verbs
Action verbs
Adverbs
Relative adverbs
Interrogative adverbs
Adverbs of time
Adverbs of place
Adverbs of reason
Adverbs of quantity
Adverbs of manner
Adverbs of frequency
Adverbs of affirmation
Adjectives
Quantitative adjective
Proper adjective
Possessive adjective
Numeral adjective
Interrogative adjective
Distributive adjective
Descriptive adjective
Demonstrative adjective
Pronouns
Subject pronoun
Relative pronoun
Reflexive pronoun
Reciprocal pronoun
Possessive pronoun
Personal pronoun
Interrogative pronoun
Indefinite pronoun
Emphatic pronoun
Distributive pronoun
Demonstrative pronoun
Pre Position
Preposition by function
Time preposition
Reason preposition
Possession preposition
Place preposition
Phrases preposition
Origin preposition
Measure preposition
Direction preposition
Contrast preposition
Agent preposition
Preposition by construction
Simple preposition
Phrase preposition
Double preposition
Compound preposition
Conjunctions
Subordinating conjunction
Correlative conjunction
Coordinating conjunction
Conjunctive adverbs
Interjections
Express calling interjection
Grammar Rules
Preference
Requests and offers
wishes
Be used to
Some and any
Could have done
Describing people
Giving advices
Possession
Comparative and superlative
Giving Reason
Making Suggestions
Apologizing
Forming questions
Since and for
Directions
Obligation
Adverbials
invitation
Articles
Imaginary condition
Zero conditional
First conditional
Second conditional
Third conditional
Reported speech
Linguistics
Phonetics
Phonology
Semantics
Pragmatics
Linguistics fields
Syntax
Morphology
Semantics
pragmatics
History
Writing
Grammar
Phonetics and Phonology
Reading Comprehension
Elementary
Intermediate
Advanced
Truncation and stress
المؤلف: Ingo Plag
المصدر: Morphological Productivity
الجزء والصفحة: P164-C6
2025-02-03
70
Truncation and stress
The next problematic point concerning the phonology of -ize is truncation. It is clear that possible bases of -ize may lose final segments, but we have seen that it is still unclear how this can be restricted. Raffelsiefen proposes two auxiliary constraints to account for truncation restrictions. The first one, already mentioned above, dictates that two syllables of the base must remain intact, the second one "requires all onsets in the base to correspond to identical onsets in the derived form" (Raffelsiefen 1996:200, note 19). Unfortunately, these constraints are not discussed in detail in her article, and it is left unclear how they should be incorporated. However, our earlier remarks have already shown that the first constraint is inadequate. The second one is more promising, however.
It can be observed that only entire word-final rhymes and not just word-final consonants may be truncated. In Raffelsiefen's model the deletion of a single consonantal coda is made impossible by other constraints, like *VV, which is evoked to rule out forms like *emphasiize. However, it is still left unclear as to why the final segment in complex codas is never deleted (cf. the discussion of words like potentize/*potenize above). In a preliminary fashion these facts can be generalized as in (1):
(1)
We may now turn to the stress clash constraint. Both Goldsmith (1990:270ff) and Raffelsiefen (1996) point out that stress clashes are generally avoided in possible words.1 So far, we have frequently referred to this constraint, but, in view of the numerous counterexamples, its exact formulation and status remain problematic.
I assume that we have to distinguish three degrees of stress in English. Syllables may have primary stress, non-primary (i.e. secondary) stress,2 or no stress (cf., e.g., Giegerich 1985). This trichotomy is corroborated by both phonological and phonetic facts (see Giegerich 1992). The stress behavior of -ize derivatives can now be largely predicted from the segmental structure of the suffix. Since the syllabic structure of -ize involves a branching rhyme with a diphthong, the suffix will attract secondary stress.3 Given that stress clashes tend to be avoided in complex words, we can predict that -ize usually does not occur after stressed syllables. Counterexamples like banálìze and routìnìze,4* are, however, occasionally coined, which shows that speakers may violate this constraint if no other possibility is available to avoid the clash (for example through truncation). Although the stress clash constraint seems to be important and necessary, Raffelsiefen cannot account for some derivatives like ràdiòìze which are impossible formations in her model, because they violate both *VV and * CLASH, whereas the corresponding truncated form *rádiìze would only violate *VV and the lower ranked IDENT constraint. Again, the con straints as proposed by Raffelsiefen do not make the correct predictions.
Another stress-related problem is that with some isolated forms involving secondary ultimate stress, -ize attachment may lead to the destressing of secondarily stressed syllables. Consider the following pairs:
(2)
[ɪ]5
Similar alternations are discussed by Kettemann who proposes individual rules of vowel reduction ([ɑ] ↔ [ə], [aɪ] ↔ [ə], [æ] ↔ [ə]), but explains them as instances of the Auxiliary Vowel Reduction Rule (Halle and Key ser 1971:35) in connection with SPE's Vowel Reduction Rule (1968:126). In essence, these alternations seem not to be specific to -ize derivatives but follow from general principles of stress assignment and foot construction in English (see also Giegerich 1992, Goldsmith 1990:259). In other words, the forms in (2) are more fully integrated into the general metrical system of English than forms that feature two adjacent stressed syllables. However, not all base-final secondarily stressed syllables are affected by destressing, but only those that end in a consonant. For example, ghettoize and radioize (and all other pertinent forms) preserve their secondary stress. In other words, we are faced with another incidence where the segmental structure interacts with the metrical structure in an interesting way.
Note that the syllable which carries primary stress is never reduced or destressed. This is what one would expect if stress shift were possible, but, apart from some rare exceptions, the main stress of the base word is never shifted by productive -ize attachment. There are only five forms in the corpus of 284 20th century neologisms in which the main stress of the base seems to be altered, namely bàcterìze, lyóphilìze, lysógenìze, multìmerìze, and phagocy̒tìze.
1 For Goldsmith (1990:271), the prohibition of stress clashes is dependent on an intervening word boundary. Raffelsiefen argues against this dependency on the grounds that it also holds for cases where stem allomorphy is involved, i.e. where there is only a morpheme-boundary. Furthermore, she claims that stress clashes are historically often unstable, as shown by the replacement of quantity-sensitive stress by alternating stress in words like mobile, abdomen, advertise and the like. We will return to this issue below.
2 I do not use 'secondary' in the sense of SPE stress 2, but simply as a label for syllables that have non-primary stress. For a detailed criticism of the SPE stress numerology see e.g. Hogg and McCully (1987).
3 We ignore here the possibility that -ize may receive primary stress. See below for discussion.
4 The stress patterns indicated follow the information given in the OED. Note, however, that, at least in American English, routinize is pronounced usually with primary stress on the first syllable, followed by an unstressed syllable. In other words, there is a stress shift. Note also that there are at least two dialect clusters which exhibit primary stress on -ize, Hiberno-English and Caribbean English. Dialectal variation in stress patterns of derived words in general is certainly in need of further investigation, but will not be dealt with systematically here.
5 I adopt here the pronunciations given by the OED. Some speakers prefer the pronunciation [stɹɪknaɪn] for the base word, but this does not have any effect on the pronunciation of the derived verb, because even these speakers pronounce the verb [stɹɪknɪnaɪz]. For these speakers strychninize patterns like lateritize in (2b).