Grammar
Tenses
Present
Present Simple
Present Continuous
Present Perfect
Present Perfect Continuous
Past
Past Continuous
Past Perfect
Past Perfect Continuous
Past Simple
Future
Future Simple
Future Continuous
Future Perfect
Future Perfect Continuous
Passive and Active
Parts Of Speech
Nouns
Countable and uncountable nouns
Verbal nouns
Singular and Plural nouns
Proper nouns
Nouns gender
Nouns definition
Concrete nouns
Abstract nouns
Common nouns
Collective nouns
Definition Of Nouns
Verbs
Stative and dynamic verbs
Finite and nonfinite verbs
To be verbs
Transitive and intransitive verbs
Auxiliary verbs
Modal verbs
Regular and irregular verbs
Action verbs
Adverbs
Relative adverbs
Interrogative adverbs
Adverbs of time
Adverbs of place
Adverbs of reason
Adverbs of quantity
Adverbs of manner
Adverbs of frequency
Adverbs of affirmation
Adjectives
Quantitative adjective
Proper adjective
Possessive adjective
Numeral adjective
Interrogative adjective
Distributive adjective
Descriptive adjective
Demonstrative adjective
Pronouns
Subject pronoun
Relative pronoun
Reflexive pronoun
Reciprocal pronoun
Possessive pronoun
Personal pronoun
Interrogative pronoun
Indefinite pronoun
Emphatic pronoun
Distributive pronoun
Demonstrative pronoun
Pre Position
Preposition by function
Time preposition
Reason preposition
Possession preposition
Place preposition
Phrases preposition
Origin preposition
Measure preposition
Direction preposition
Contrast preposition
Agent preposition
Preposition by construction
Simple preposition
Phrase preposition
Double preposition
Compound preposition
Conjunctions
Subordinating conjunction
Correlative conjunction
Coordinating conjunction
Conjunctive adverbs
Interjections
Express calling interjection
Grammar Rules
Preference
Requests and offers
wishes
Be used to
Some and any
Could have done
Describing people
Giving advices
Possession
Comparative and superlative
Giving Reason
Making Suggestions
Apologizing
Forming questions
Since and for
Directions
Obligation
Adverbials
invitation
Articles
Imaginary condition
Zero conditional
First conditional
Second conditional
Third conditional
Reported speech
Linguistics
Phonetics
Phonology
Semantics
Pragmatics
Linguistics fields
Syntax
Morphology
Semantics
pragmatics
History
Writing
Grammar
Phonetics and Phonology
Reading Comprehension
Elementary
Intermediate
Advanced
The combinability of derivational suffixes Conclusion
المؤلف: Ingo Plag
المصدر: Morphological Productivity
الجزء والصفحة: P90-C4
2025-01-17
162
The combinability of derivational suffixes Conclusion
It has been argued that Fabb's generalization concerning English suffixation, expressed in the title of his article, is substantially flawed, both empirically and theoretically.
The analysis of a large amount of data has demonstrated that there is an abundance of counterexamples to many of Fabb's claims. Although it is a well-known fact that lexical rules are often subject to exceptions, this point cannot be evoked to explain away the numerous and often systematically occurring patterns since in Fabb's framework we are not dealing with rules, but with selectional restrictions, i.e. idiosyncratic information of individual lexical items (in this case: suffixes). Thus, even if we accept Fabb's analysis, the counterexamples are not exceptions to proposed rules but violations of lexical requirements as stated in the individual suffix's lexical entry. And systematic violations of such requirements are a rather unexpected phenomenon, to say the least.
In my view, however, the most important arguments against Fabb's proposed selectional restrictions are of a conceptual nature. Thus, even if we did not count the numerous "exceptions", many, if not all failures of suffixes to attach to already suffixed forms are the natural consequence of phonological, morphological, semantic, and pragmatic constraints that have to be stated anyway in a reasonably adequate description of English suffixation. These constraints, in particular base-driven selectional restrictions and general morphological constraints like the Latinate Constraint and blocking, regulate the applicability of derivational processes to given domains, ruling out a great many logically possible combinations of stems and affixes. The kinds of selectional restrictions proposed by Fabb have been shown to be either empirically and theoretically inadequate or simply superfluous.
From the discussion of Fabb's last group of suffixes one can conclude that generalizations like "affixes that do not attach outside any affix/that attach outside one other affix" are of no theoretical significance. The fact that there are suffixes which share the property that they only attach to one or to two other suffixes does not make them a natural class, nor a morphologically relevant one. In fact, according to the restrictions that apply across the board, we would have to posit groups of suffixes that attach outside three, four, five, six, seven, and so forth, suffixes. In the present account the number of possible preceding suffixes is a mere artefact of the phonological, morphological, and semantic-pragmatic constraints at work, and thus theoretically insignificant.
The rejection of Fabb's proposal has wider theoretical implications. One of these implications concerns the problem whether word-internal structure is accessible to morphological processes. As was mentioned above, Fabb's model rests crucially on the assumption that suffixes are sensitive to the morphological make-up of potential bases. The rejection of Fabb's selectional restrictions does, however, not entail that we argue against the accessibility of word-internal structure to morphological processes. The approach taken here is rather neutral with regard to this controversy, although some of the processes discussed (e.g. -ment suffixation to prefixed bases) seem to involve access to the morphological make-up of the base word. At least some morphological processes are best explained by assuming access to word-internal structure (Carstairs-McCarthy 1993).
Future research will have to determine when word-internal structure is crucially visible and when it is not.
Finally, we have made some potentially controversial claims concerning the nature of selectional restrictions in morphology. For many of the suffixes it was shown that the incapability of a given suffix to attach to a certain class of stems was not due to some assumed property of the suffix, but rather due to a crucial property of the putative base. Taking this point seriously means to question any strictly affix-driven approach as put forward by standard lexical morphology or other stratum-oriented approaches. Most recently, Giegerich (1998) has proposed a base-driven stratificational model of English morphology, in which he argues against affix-driven morphology on different grounds. His idea of the bas-drivenness of morphological processes is independently supported by our findings. It seems, however, that we do not need a stratum model to account for the intriguing phenomena.
The foregoing presentation was designed as a tour d'horizon through English suffixation, giving an outline of what I consider the most important structural mechanisms that restrict the productivity of derivational processes. The following subjects will offer a more microscopic view of the matter through an in-depth analysis of verb-deriving processes in English.