Grammar
Tenses
Present
Present Simple
Present Continuous
Present Perfect
Present Perfect Continuous
Past
Past Continuous
Past Perfect
Past Perfect Continuous
Past Simple
Future
Future Simple
Future Continuous
Future Perfect
Future Perfect Continuous
Passive and Active
Parts Of Speech
Nouns
Countable and uncountable nouns
Verbal nouns
Singular and Plural nouns
Proper nouns
Nouns gender
Nouns definition
Concrete nouns
Abstract nouns
Common nouns
Collective nouns
Definition Of Nouns
Verbs
Stative and dynamic verbs
Finite and nonfinite verbs
To be verbs
Transitive and intransitive verbs
Auxiliary verbs
Modal verbs
Regular and irregular verbs
Action verbs
Adverbs
Relative adverbs
Interrogative adverbs
Adverbs of time
Adverbs of place
Adverbs of reason
Adverbs of quantity
Adverbs of manner
Adverbs of frequency
Adverbs of affirmation
Adjectives
Quantitative adjective
Proper adjective
Possessive adjective
Numeral adjective
Interrogative adjective
Distributive adjective
Descriptive adjective
Demonstrative adjective
Pronouns
Subject pronoun
Relative pronoun
Reflexive pronoun
Reciprocal pronoun
Possessive pronoun
Personal pronoun
Interrogative pronoun
Indefinite pronoun
Emphatic pronoun
Distributive pronoun
Demonstrative pronoun
Pre Position
Preposition by function
Time preposition
Reason preposition
Possession preposition
Place preposition
Phrases preposition
Origin preposition
Measure preposition
Direction preposition
Contrast preposition
Agent preposition
Preposition by construction
Simple preposition
Phrase preposition
Double preposition
Compound preposition
Conjunctions
Subordinating conjunction
Correlative conjunction
Coordinating conjunction
Conjunctive adverbs
Interjections
Express calling interjection
Grammar Rules
Preference
Requests and offers
wishes
Be used to
Some and any
Could have done
Describing people
Giving advices
Possession
Comparative and superlative
Giving Reason
Making Suggestions
Apologizing
Forming questions
Since and for
Directions
Obligation
Adverbials
invitation
Articles
Imaginary condition
Zero conditional
First conditional
Second conditional
Third conditional
Reported speech
Linguistics
Phonetics
Phonology
Semantics
Pragmatics
Linguistics fields
Syntax
Morphology
Semantics
pragmatics
History
Writing
Grammar
Phonetics and Phonology
Reading Comprehension
Elementary
Intermediate
Advanced
The ordering of SVLR and LLL in a Lexical Phonology
المؤلف: APRIL McMAHON
المصدر: LEXICAL PHONOLOGY AND THE HISTORY OF ENGLISH
الجزء والصفحة: P191-C4
2024-12-20
73
The ordering of SVLR and LLL in a Lexical Phonology
Various criteria for distinguishing lexical from postlexical rule applications are suggested by, for instance, Kiparsky (1982) and Mohanan (1982, 1986); a subset is given in (1).
(1)
Although, as we saw, there is reason to believe that the divide between lexical and postlexical rule applications is more of a cline than a rigid and unbridgeable division, there is some support for these criteria. For instance, although some postlexical rule applications, such as glottalling in English, are markers or even stereotypes, most are automatic phonetic processes, like aspiration of voiceless stops in English, and native speakers fail to observe their effects. It seems that LLL meets this criterion; according to Delattre (1962: 1142) `Some speakers will make a distinctive difference of length between bomb and balm, but they will make a larger difference of length - though non-distinctive - between leap and leave. And the naive subject will easily be made conscious of the first difference of length but not the second.'
However, Scots/SSE speakers do seem to be generally aware of the differences produced by SVLR (or can easily be made aware of them). SVLR thus appears to control a binary, categorizable distinction of length; LLL, on the other hand, increases the duration of long and short vowels by a variable amount, depending on the nature of the following consonant: its output is therefore essentially non-binary.
Mohanan's major criterion for distinguishing between lexical and postlexical rules involves sensitivity to the morphology: `A rule application requiring morphological information must take place in the lexicon' (1986: 9). LLL might initially seem to be lexical by this criterion, since sensitivity to morphological information would include sensitivity to the presence of boundaries, and vowels are lengthened word-finally. However, it seems that LLL affects vowels utterance-finally, or prepausally, rather than word-finally; if pauses are inserted after syntactic concatenation (Mohanan 1982), any rule referring to the position of pauses is necessarily postlexical. SVLR, on the other hand, is clearly sensitive to morphological information, and indeed a boundary is included in its structural description. SVLR lengthens vowels word finally, but also before regular inflections, even when the consonant following the boundary is not itself a lengthening context; the vowel is therefore lengthened in sees [si:z] and keyed [ki:d], and in brewed and tied but not brood and tide.
LLL can therefore be classified as clearly postlexical, and SVLR as tenuously lexical. SVLR operates when the affected vowel is stem-final and in a Class II derived or regularly inflected form, or in the first stem of a compound, but not in morphologically underived forms with similar phonological contexts; relevant data (from Harris 1989) are shown in (2). I argued that Class II derivation, regular inflection and compounding all take place on Level 2; so must SVLR.
(2)
Carr (1992) presents three types of evidence which may indicate that SVLR also applies on Level 1. First, in Scots and SSE the vowel in ablaut past tense forms like rode, strode (as opposed to road) is long: Carr argues that Level 1 ablaut rules, in the style of Halle and Mohanan (1985) provide a derived environment for lengthening in these cases. Secondly, Noun Plural Fricative Voicing is said to feed SVLR on Level 1, in life ~ lives, leaf ~ leaves and hoof ~ hooves. Finally, Carr cites the cases in (3), from Allan (1985).
(3)
The ablaut and Noun Plural Fricative Voicing examples are variable, and a pattern of length attributable to SVLR is only observable for some speakers; this, however, is what one might expect of Level 1 rules, which seem to control alternations mid-way between the fully productive and the fully lexicalized.
Carr (1992) argues that, although SVLR applies at Level 1 in ablaut past tenses and Noun Plural Fricative Voicing cases; in general lengthening takes place precyclically on Level 2, before affixation. Thus, in row, rowed, SVLR applies at Level 2 in open syllables: Carr (1992) prefers this context to the following boundary ] assumed above since his version will include cases like spider, pylon, where McMahon (1991) has to assume reanalysis with a `false' morpheme boundary for those speakers with lengthened vowels.
However, very little of Carr's evidence allows clear conclusions to be drawn on the level ordering of SVLR. First, I have argued above that ablaut past tenses in Modern English should not be derived by rule, even on Level 1; instead, all those outside the keep ~ kept class should be treated as having two lexical entries. In that case, forms like rode, strode do not constitute Level 1 derived environments for SVLR, and must be stored with a long vowel underlyingly. In historical terms, their long vowel is the result of analogy with regular past tense forms like rowed, snowed, which are eligible for SVLR on Level 2. I shall argue that this operation of analogy was partly responsible for the extension of SVLR from Level 2 to Level 1; and indeed, this account predicts that for some time, when ablaut was still a semi-productive process, it would have fed SVLR in these forms on Level 1. Now, however, we have fossilized and stored alternations, and part of that storage involves the vowel length historically attributable to SVLR. Carr (1992: 104±5) claims that invoking analogy in this way is non explanatory, and tantamount to accepting that SVLR does apply in these forms. I disagree: it is tantamount to accepting that SVLR did apply in these forms, when its conditions were met by the application of a preceding process on Level 1. The demise of that process for Modern English speakers means that although the effects of historical SVLR are still discernible in cases like rode, they are now part of a learned alternation. Anderson (1993: 425) also points out that ablaut per se cannot be responsible for feeding SVLR in these cases, as Carr claims, since lengthening takes place only before the past tense marker-d (and not in wrote, therefore). Since SVLR in regular past tenses always involves a vowel-final stem, which in the normal course of events will attract a following past marker-d, this supports my proposal of analogy between rowed and rode, rather than a (semi-) productive connection with ablaut.
Secondly, and perhaps surprisingly, Carr does not use Allan's (1985) evidence in (3) above to support his proposal of Level 1 SVLR. Allan argues that the advise ~ advice cases represent Verb to Noun zero derivation, which according to Kiparsky (1982) is a Level 1 process. However, if this is so, Carr's precyclic SVLR would counterfactually lengthen both forms. Carr is therefore forced to analyze these as denominal zero derivations, which Kiparsky (1982) assigns to Level 2; but his only grounds for so doing are that half ~ halve and mouth ~ mouthe `look more plausibly like cases of N ? V zero derivation', and that this can automatically be extended to the advise ~ advice alternations. Even so, Carr must then allow SVLR to apply on Level 2 in open syllables, and on Levels 1 and 2 before voiced continuants, the only apparent advantage being the lack of reference to ]. But again, there are two objections. Carr's assumption of open-syllable SVLR does not seem well motivated: there are speakers who lengthen the vowel in pylon, spider, but equally there are others who do not, and yet another group who lengthen one but not the other. The evidence above from Milroy (1995) on SVLR in Newcastle and Donegan (1993) on Canadian Raising also suggest that exceptionally long and short diphthongs occur fairly frequently, and that perhaps a split of /Λi/ from /ai/ is in progress. Furthermore, the use of ] in phonological rules reflects the interaction of morphology and phonology which is central to LP: of course it is important to allow for purely phonological conditioning, but it is equally vital to recognize cases where morphological factors are paramount, and allow the phonology to refer to these. I shall argue in 4.6 that the historical reanalysis of SVLR from LLL necessarily involves both analogy and reference to ], and that the incipient contrast of /Λi/ and /ai/ is also central to the continued development of SVLR. I therefore maintain the formulation of SVLR suggested in (The environment for SVLR (5)): SVLR will apply on Levels 1 and 2; but in the former case, will be restricted by the Derived Environment Condition.