Grammar
Tenses
Present
Present Simple
Present Continuous
Present Perfect
Present Perfect Continuous
Past
Past Simple
Past Continuous
Past Perfect
Past Perfect Continuous
Future
Future Simple
Future Continuous
Future Perfect
Future Perfect Continuous
Parts Of Speech
Nouns
Countable and uncountable nouns
Verbal nouns
Singular and Plural nouns
Proper nouns
Nouns gender
Nouns definition
Concrete nouns
Abstract nouns
Common nouns
Collective nouns
Definition Of Nouns
Verbs
Stative and dynamic verbs
Finite and nonfinite verbs
To be verbs
Transitive and intransitive verbs
Auxiliary verbs
Modal verbs
Regular and irregular verbs
Action verbs
Adverbs
Relative adverbs
Interrogative adverbs
Adverbs of time
Adverbs of place
Adverbs of reason
Adverbs of quantity
Adverbs of manner
Adverbs of frequency
Adverbs of affirmation
Adjectives
Quantitative adjective
Proper adjective
Possessive adjective
Numeral adjective
Interrogative adjective
Distributive adjective
Descriptive adjective
Demonstrative adjective
Pronouns
Subject pronoun
Relative pronoun
Reflexive pronoun
Reciprocal pronoun
Possessive pronoun
Personal pronoun
Interrogative pronoun
Indefinite pronoun
Emphatic pronoun
Distributive pronoun
Demonstrative pronoun
Pre Position
Preposition by function
Time preposition
Reason preposition
Possession preposition
Place preposition
Phrases preposition
Origin preposition
Measure preposition
Direction preposition
Contrast preposition
Agent preposition
Preposition by construction
Simple preposition
Phrase preposition
Double preposition
Compound preposition
Conjunctions
Subordinating conjunction
Correlative conjunction
Coordinating conjunction
Conjunctive adverbs
Interjections
Express calling interjection
Grammar Rules
Passive and Active
Preference
Requests and offers
wishes
Be used to
Some and any
Could have done
Describing people
Giving advices
Possession
Comparative and superlative
Giving Reason
Making Suggestions
Apologizing
Forming questions
Since and for
Directions
Obligation
Adverbials
invitation
Articles
Imaginary condition
Zero conditional
First conditional
Second conditional
Third conditional
Reported speech
Linguistics
Phonetics
Phonology
Linguistics fields
Syntax
Morphology
Semantics
pragmatics
History
Writing
Grammar
Phonetics and Phonology
Semiotics
Reading Comprehension
Elementary
Intermediate
Advanced
Teaching Methods
Teaching Strategies
Assessment
KNOWLEDGE
المؤلف:
John Field
المصدر:
Psycholinguistics
الجزء والصفحة:
P141
2025-09-07
87
KNOWLEDGE
Linguistic knowledge takes the form of a number of dichotomies:
Declarative/procedural. Declarative knowledge (knowledge that) is explicit and can be expressed verbally. It contrasts with procedural knowledge (knowledge how) which determines our ability to handle highly automatic routines such as retrieving words or constructing sentences in our first language. Declarative knowledge involves control: it demands attentional resources and is therefore costly in terms of working memory capacity.
Declarative knowledge can be transformed into procedural knowledge through practice. This leads (a) to separate steps becoming combined and (b) to ever-increasing automaticity. The declarative/ procedural distinction has been used in accounts of second language acquisition to describe the way in which some learners move from rule-based information to the relatively automatic generation of utterances.
Implicit/explicit. Implicit linguistic knowledge manifests itself in performance but the possessor is unable to express it verbally. In second language acquisition, a system of phonology or syntax acquired in a naturalistic (i.e. non-instructional) environment might be stored by means of a mapping between a particular form and a particular context, with no explicit rule attached. Similarly, inductive teaching might present the learner with a typical set of circumstances in which a structure is to be used, without expressing these conditions of use in the form of a rule. In these cases, inferencing is necessary on the part of the user in order to turn implicit knowledge into explicit.
The implicit/explicit distinction is also important in research into first language processes. Certain traditional psycholinguistic tasks draw upon explicit knowledge: requiring subjects, for example, to memorise lists of words, and thus store explicit information. Today psycholinguists increasingly prefer to use indirect or incidental tasks. They might, for example, demonstrate that a reader reads a word more quickly as a result of having been exposed to it recently, even though the reader cannot specifically recall having seen the word.
Categorical/probabilistic. Some linguistic knowledge is categorical in form, enabling the possessor to classify material on an ‘all or none’ basis. An example is the way in which a sharp and consistent boundary is maintained between a set of phonetic exemplars which are perceived as representing one consonant and a set perceived as representing a contrasted one. Other types of linguistic knowledge are probabilistic and operate on the basis of ‘best fit’. This might permit us to recognise lexical items that have been subject to assimilation (e.g. ten pronounced [tem] in the sequence ten pounds).
Formulaic/analysed. Many infants first acquire and produce whole chunks of language in a formulaic way, without being able to analyse them into their constituents. Conversely, a minority appear to acquire speech analytically (word by word). They then have to assemble the words into chunks which are integrated phonologically and can be stored as pre-assembled lexical units. Second language learners in a ‘naturalistic’ context are more likely to acquire language formulaically, while those learning in a classroom setting acquire language in a more analysed form.
See also: ACT, Analysis, Categorical perception, Implicit learning, Incidental learning, Probabilistic
Further reading: Kellogg (1995: Chap. 6)
الاكثر قراءة في Linguistics fields
اخر الاخبار
اخبار العتبة العباسية المقدسة

الآخبار الصحية
