1

المرجع الالكتروني للمعلوماتية

Grammar

Tenses

Present

Present Simple

Present Continuous

Present Perfect

Present Perfect Continuous

Past

Past Continuous

Past Perfect

Past Perfect Continuous

Past Simple

Future

Future Simple

Future Continuous

Future Perfect

Future Perfect Continuous

Passive and Active

Parts Of Speech

Nouns

Countable and uncountable nouns

Verbal nouns

Singular and Plural nouns

Proper nouns

Nouns gender

Nouns definition

Concrete nouns

Abstract nouns

Common nouns

Collective nouns

Definition Of Nouns

Verbs

Stative and dynamic verbs

Finite and nonfinite verbs

To be verbs

Transitive and intransitive verbs

Auxiliary verbs

Modal verbs

Regular and irregular verbs

Action verbs

Adverbs

Relative adverbs

Interrogative adverbs

Adverbs of time

Adverbs of place

Adverbs of reason

Adverbs of quantity

Adverbs of manner

Adverbs of frequency

Adverbs of affirmation

Adjectives

Quantitative adjective

Proper adjective

Possessive adjective

Numeral adjective

Interrogative adjective

Distributive adjective

Descriptive adjective

Demonstrative adjective

Pronouns

Subject pronoun

Relative pronoun

Reflexive pronoun

Reciprocal pronoun

Possessive pronoun

Personal pronoun

Interrogative pronoun

Indefinite pronoun

Emphatic pronoun

Distributive pronoun

Demonstrative pronoun

Pre Position

Preposition by function

Time preposition

Reason preposition

Possession preposition

Place preposition

Phrases preposition

Origin preposition

Measure preposition

Direction preposition

Contrast preposition

Agent preposition

Preposition by construction

Simple preposition

Phrase preposition

Double preposition

Compound preposition

Conjunctions

Subordinating conjunction

Correlative conjunction

Coordinating conjunction

Conjunctive adverbs

Interjections

Express calling interjection

Grammar Rules

Preference

Requests and offers

wishes

Be used to

Some and any

Could have done

Describing people

Giving advices

Possession

Comparative and superlative

Giving Reason

Making Suggestions

Apologizing

Forming questions

Since and for

Directions

Obligation

Adverbials

invitation

Articles

Imaginary condition

Zero conditional

First conditional

Second conditional

Third conditional

Reported speech

Linguistics

Phonetics

Phonology

Semantics

Pragmatics

Linguistics fields

Syntax

Morphology

Semantics

pragmatics

History

Writing

Grammar

Phonetics and Phonology

Reading Comprehension

Elementary

Intermediate

Advanced

English Language : Linguistics : Syntax :

The evolution of generative grammar

المؤلف:  David Hornsby

المصدر:  Linguistics A complete introduction

الجزء والصفحة:  162-8

2023-12-25

928

The evolution of generative grammar

Unlike the Descriptivists, who built their grammars ‘upwards’ from phonemes to sentences, Chomsky put syntax at the centre of his formal model, providing rules to generate well-formed sequences from abstract syntactic units. The essential components of a sentence (S) of traditional grammar, namely subject and predicate, were reframed in these terms as NP and VP:

S NP VP  

 

These two constituents might then be rewritten as follows:

NP (Det) (Adj) N

VP V (NP)

In this rule notation, the bracketed items are optional but the non-bracketed ones are not: a noun phrase and a verb phrase must be headed by a noun and a verb respectively. Our simple NP rewrite rule generates such phrases as the old man, this house and girls, and our sentence rule generates a very large number of sentences, including Chomsky’s own example from Syntactic Structures:

The man hit the ball.

 

which we can present either as a labelled bracket structure S [ NP[ Det[The] N[Man]] VP[V[hit] NP [Det[the] N[ball]] or, more commonly, for ease of exposition, as a tree diagram:

 

To generate an infinite number of grammatical sentences from finite means, the model has to allow for recursion, which is achieved by allowing constituents to occur within constituents of the same kind. In the example below, for example, S recurs as a daughter node of VP:

 

Recursions of this kind illustrate an important difference between what Chomsky calls competence, an individual’s internalized grammar, and performance, its realization in speech. The capacity to produce infinitely long recursive sentences is a matter of competence, but limits are imposed by real world considerations of performance: overlong sentences are boring and difficult to process, you have a finite amount of breath, and your interlocutor may do you an injury if you do not stop after a reasonable amount of time.

 

The partial grammar of English above rules out sentences that do not conform to its phrase-structure (PS) rules, for example:

1 *Clever girl the questions answered

2 *Exists house

3 *The brown cat ate yellow

 

It would, however, require some refinement in order not to generate ungrammatical sentences like the following:

4 This green cats eat a mice

5 John exists a banana

 

Modifications to the grammar, for example agreement rules in (4) and a specification in the lexicon that exist cannot take an object NP in (5), are easily introduced. But Chomsky draws our attention to a more fundamental problem of phrase-structure grammars, namely that they fail to account for relationships between sentences, for example this active/ passive pair:

1 The cat ate the mouse

2 The mouse was eaten by the cat

 

The relatedness of the this pair is not evident from their structural description, but Chomsky argues that the passive (7) is derived from the active (6) by what at this stage he calls a transformation, which he sets out thus:

‘If S1 is a grammatical sentence of the form

NP1 – Aux – V – NP2

then the corresponding string of the form

NP2 - Aux + be + en1 – V – by + NP1

is also a grammatical sentence.’

 

By positing a transformational component allowing constructions in surface structure to be derived from others in deep structure, Chomsky accounts for complex structural ambiguities. In (1) below, for example, the structural ambiguity comes from the different constructions in deep structure (2) and (3) from which (1) is derived:

1 The shooting of the hunters

2 The hunters shot (someone/something)

3 (Someone/something) shot the hunters

Note that, in early models of generative grammar, surface structure is not to be equated with output: the latter is generated from surface structure by the phonological component.

EN

تصفح الموقع بالشكل العمودي